Psychology

How Positivism Shaped Our Understanding of Reality

How Positivism Shaped Our Understanding of Reality

Portrait of Auguste Comte, symbolizing the beginnings of positivism.

Auguste Comte, the father of positivism, whose ideas revolutionized our understanding of science and reality.

Introduction

Definition of Positivism

Positivism is a philosophical approach asserting that authentic knowledge is derived from sensory experience, preferably through scientific methods. In its essence, positivism advocates that our knowledge of reality is firmly rooted in observable and measurable phenomena rather than abstract theorization or metaphysical speculation.

Brief Historical Context

The term positivism traces its origins to the early nineteenth century, significantly influenced by thinkers such as Auguste Comte and Jeremy Bentham. According to the Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy, Comte, often termed the father of sociology, posited that human societies progress in stages, with the scientific or "positive" stage being the pinnacle.

Concurrently, Bentham, alongside John Stuart Mill, laid the groundwork for "legal positivism," distinguishing between the law as it exists and what it ought to be, which contrasts with the natural law doctrine. Moreover, with figures like De Saint Simon advocating for a new social order rooted in scientific reasoning, the emergence of positivism fundamentally shaped the course of intellectual history.

Importance of the Concept in the Development of Scientific Thinking

The rise of positivism in the nineteenth century marked a decisive shift in how knowledge was pursued and validated. By advocating scientific methods, positivism played an instrumental role in the evolution of the social sciences. In the realm of natural sciences, positivism's emphasis on empirical evidence fostered rigorous methodologies and objective analysis.

Furthermore, logical positivism, which emerged from the Vienna Circle, argued that meaningful statements must either be empirically verifiable or analytically true, further refining the positivist approach. This thought stream influenced many academic institutions, with notable publications from Oxford University Press and Cambridge University Press contributing to its dissemination and discussion.

The impact of positivism extends beyond academia. The motto "Ordem e Progresso" (Order and Progress) on Brazil's flag embodies positivist principles, underscoring the concept's influence in shaping national identities and advocating for a social order based on reason and empirical knowledge.

Historical Development of Positivism

Early Roots: Auguste Comte and the Foundation of Positivism

The records of history regard Auguste Comte as a revolutionary thinker, especially in the context of positivism. Born amidst the upheavals of post-revolutionary France, the surrounding chaos shaped Comte's perspectives. Seeking order and reason, he laid down the philosophy of positivism, emphasizing the necessity of empirical evidence in human knowledge.

Comte's positivism diverged from traditional epistemologies, emphasizing that true knowledge emanated from observed facts and the relationships among them. In the intricate tapestry of human intellectual development, Comte introduced the idea of societies evolving through three distinct stages: the theological, the metaphysical, and the positive. The final stage—positive—represented a society where knowledge is rooted in scientific methods and empirical inquiries.

The Evolution of Positivist Thinking in the 19th and Early 20th Century

The 19th century was pivotal for positivist thinking. An age of revolutions, both industrial and political, the era demanded rigorous methodologies to understand the rapidly changing world. Enter Jeremy Bentham, whose work in legal studies was transformative.

Bentham's legal positivism stipulated that laws are human creations designed to serve the collective good. This was revolutionary, as it redefined the foundation of legal systems, grounding them in human intent and consensus rather than divine ordination.

Equally influential was De Saint Simon, who envisaged a society steered by positivist principles. In his utopia, societies would dispense with outdated superstitions and biases, instead embracing a framework rooted in scientific knowledge and empirical evidence. It was a compelling vision of a world where science and rationality would craft a harmonious social order.

Key Figures and Their Contributions

The 20th century saw positivism's principles refined and debated fiercely. Central to this discourse was the Vienna Circle, a group of philosophers and scientists who pioneered logical positivism. Rudolf Carnap, a key figure in this movement, argued passionately that meaningful statements should be empirically verifiable or logically consistent. For Carnap and his peers, this was not just a theoretical exercise but a call to arms against metaphysical obscurantism.

However, positivism's influence was not confined to the European mainland. The venerable institutions of Oxford and Cambridge in the UK became hotbeds for rigorous discussions on positivism. Through their prestigious publishing arms—Oxford University Press and Cambridge University Press—both universities endorsed and critiqued positivist methodologies, testifying to the philosophy's profound impact.

In the natural sciences, positivism's influence was nothing short of transformative. As researchers delved into the mysteries of atoms and galaxies, they adhered to the rigorous empirical methods backed by positivism. In the domain of social sciences, the philosophy found resonance among researchers keen to study societies and individuals through quantifiable and observable data, sidestepping unfounded speculation.

By the end of the 20th century, while positivism faced critiques and modifications, its legacy was undeniable. It had indelibly shaped multiple disciplines, from law to sociology to natural sciences, molding the modern scientific temperament.

Key Principles of Positivism

The Primacy of Observation and Empirical Methods

Positivism's foundational pillar is the belief that knowledge emanates primarily from observation and empirical methods. This conviction has its roots in the perspective that proper understanding of the world around us requires tangible, sensory-driven evidence, as opposed to abstract reasoning or speculation. It is not merely enough to theorize or philosophize; a claim to earn the positivist's trust must be demonstrable and observable.

For instance, a positivist would not just accept anecdotal experiences or ancient texts as evidence if one were to make a claim about a specific medical condition. They would demand recent clinical trials, patient observations, and concrete data. This rigorous insistence on empirical evidence has directly influenced the methodologies of modern scientific research, ensuring that conclusions drawn are based on direct or indirect observations of the world.

Rejection of Metaphysics and Speculation

Another central tenet of positivism is its rejection of metaphysical assertions and speculative reasoning. This stance has made positivism somewhat controversial, especially when pitted against philosophical traditions relying heavily on metaphysics. For positivists, if a concept or idea cannot be directly observed, measured, or tested empirically, it remains outside the domain of genuine knowledge.

Consider, for example, abstract concepts like 'soul' or 'afterlife.' While these notions hold significance in various cultural, religious, or philosophical contexts, positivism would refrain from making claims about their existence or nature since they are beyond empirical examination. This stringent boundary around what constitutes valid knowledge ensures clarity but also spurs debates about the limits of human understanding.

The Search for Universal Laws

The ambition to uncover universal laws, inspired by the success of the natural sciences, underscores positivism's approach. Just as physicists might seek laws governing physical phenomena, a positivist sociologist aims to discern patterns or laws in human behavior. For instance, while individual human actions might seem spontaneous or unpredictable, positivists believe that, when observed in large numbers, these actions might reveal consistent patterns, reflecting underlying societal laws.

This principle often translates into large-scale studies or surveys in the social sciences, where patterns emerge from vast amounts of data. The belief is that, much like gravity acts consistently on falling objects, there might be 'laws' of human interaction waiting to be discovered.

The Use of Inductive Reasoning

Closely tied to the emphasis on observation is the reliance on inductive reasoning. It is a process that begins with specific observations and measures, subsequently moving towards broader generalizations or theories. After accumulating sufficient data on a specific phenomenon, a positivist would seek to understand the broader implications or patterns underlying that data.

Imagine a researcher studying the dietary habits of a specific population. After collecting detailed data on individual eating habits, they might use inductive reasoning to make broader statements about dietary trends or health implications for that entire population.

Logical Positivism

Introduction and Origins (Vienna Circle)

In the pulsating intellectual milieu of the early 20th century, the Vienna Circle, a consortium of forward-thinking philosophers, mathematicians, and scientists, concretized the foundations of logical positivism. Operating mainly in the 1920s and 1930s, this group did not merely coalesce to engage in academic exercises. Instead, they were on a mission: to conceptualize an approach to philosophy underscored by empirical science and formal logic. Their commitment was palpable, leading to regular meetings and rigorous discussions.

Ludwig Wittgenstein's "Tractatus Logico-Philosophicus" was essential among the myriad influences shaping the group's thinking. This groundbreaking work was lauded for maintaining that propositions are meaningful only if they are empirically verifiable or logically deducible. The implications were vast, suggesting that a considerable portion of historical, philosophical discourse was, in fact, baseless.

The Central Tenets: Verification Principle, Language Analysis

While rooted in the broader philosophy of positivism, logical positivism introduced fresh nuances to the discourse. The Verification Principle emerged as its most distinctive tenet. This rigorous principle advanced the belief that for a statement to possess meaning, it had to be either tautological or empirically verifiable.

In simpler terms, if a claim did not hold true by its inherent definition (akin to mathematical or logical assertions) or could not be verified empirically, it was rendered meaningless. This revolutionary approach sought to discard vast expanses of traditional philosophy, especially metaphysical claims that evaded empirical testing.

This rigorous empirical and logical scrutiny extended beyond just philosophical claims; it encapsulated language itself. Logical positivists were convinced that the ambiguity and imprecision inherent in language were major culprits behind many of philosophy's perennial problems.

By meticulously dissecting language, they aspired to dispel these ambiguities, paving the way for more precise philosophical discussions. This convergence of positivism with linguistic analysis made logical positivism a unique sub-discipline, setting it apart from its philosophical contemporaries.

Critiques and Limitations: Problems of Verification, Neglect of Metaphysics

However, the bold assertions of logical positivism were not immune to criticism. Foremost among the critiques was a paradox within the movement's foundation. The Verification Principle, the very bedrock of logical positivism, encountered a dilemma. It was neither a self-evident truth (tautology) nor verifiable through empirical means. This meant that, ironically, the principle itself was rendered meaningless when assessed by its own standards.

Furthermore, logical positivism's emphasis on empirical verifiability inadvertently marginalized other philosophical approaches. For centuries, traditional metaphysical exploration, often criticized by logical positivists, offered profound insights into existential questions and informed the trajectory of scientific inquiries. To many, discarding metaphysics seemed a reductionist approach, ignoring the rich tapestry of human understanding and historical philosophical discourse.

Moreover, while commendable, the movement's intense focus on linguistic precision was not without issues. Critics opined that this almost obsessive attention to language had the unintended effect of sidelining pressing philosophical queries of ethics, aesthetics, and existence. Though not always fitting neatly within the empirical or logical framework supported by logical positivists, such questions were undeniably central to understanding the human experience and deserved philosophical attention.

Post Positivism

Recognizing the Limitations of Strict Positivism

Positivism, as a philosophy, reigned supreme during its pinnacle, emphasizing the necessity of observable, measurable phenomena for genuine knowledge acquisition. Its rigor and empirical stance provided a robust framework for scientific investigations. However, with the continuous evolution of philosophical thought and the increasing complexity of scientific inquiries, it became evident that a strictly positivist approach needed to be revised.

One of the primary criticisms was positivism's exclusion of anything that could not be directly observed or measured. While this empirical approach held merit in many scientific arenas, it became limiting when addressing more abstract, nuanced subject matters. For example, emotions, beliefs, and intentions, which play crucial roles in the social sciences, do not always present themselves in directly observable ways but significantly influence human behavior. Strict adherence to positivism thus inadvertently risks sidelining these pivotal aspects of human experience and understanding.

Introduction to the Concept of Falsifiability (Karl Popper)

Karl Popper's challenge to the positivist framework provided a fresh perspective on the philosophy of science. Popper argued against the idea that scientific theories could be solidified solely by accumulating confirming evidence. Instead, he introduced the concept of falsifiability, emphasizing the importance of the potential to refute theories.

Under this premise, a theory is not scientific because of the vast evidence supporting it but because it can be potentially disproven. The ability to state conditions under which a theory could be considered false makes it open to empirical testing and, thus, scientific scrutiny.

For Popper, science grows through a repetitive cycle of conjecture and refutation. This idea was revolutionary as it shifted the focus from proving theories right to proving them wrong, making scientific inquiries more rigorous and dynamic.

The Emphasis on Critical Realism

As the limitations of strict positivism became clearer, another philosophical stance, critical realism, started gaining traction. Founded on the belief that an objective reality exists, but our understanding of it is always mediated by human cognition, critical realism sought to bridge the gap between strict positivism and radical relativism.

Bhaskar, a primary proponent of critical realism, argued that some real-world mechanisms and structures operate independently of our perceptions. However, our knowledge of these mechanisms is always fallible, shaped by our tools, methodologies, and perspectives.

By recognizing this inherent fallibility, critical realism does not aim for absolute truth but instead seeks a continually refined understanding of the objective reality. This iterative approach emphasizes that while the world exists independently of our understanding, our knowledge and interpretations play a critical role in shaping our relationship with this reality.

Understanding that Knowledge is Fallible and Theory-laden

Embracing the post-positivist stance requires a nuanced understanding of the nature of knowledge. It refutes the positivist's absolute confidence in empirical findings and recognizes that all knowledge remains tentative and open to revision, no matter how rigorously obtained.

Furthermore, acknowledging that our theories and hypotheses do not emerge in a vacuum is vital. The process of scientific inquiry, rather than being purely objective, is influenced by prior knowledge and beliefs: theory-laden.

This does not discredit scientific findings but illuminates the need for continuous reflection and refinement of our method.

Thomas Kuhn's influential work highlighted this by introducing the idea of paradigm shifts in science. He argued that scientific advancements do not occur linearly but in bursts, as dominant paradigms (widely accepted scientific viewpoints) are replaced by new ones.

Kuhn emphasized that these paradigms are influenced by societal, cultural, and personal factors, reinforcing the idea that while aiming for objectivity, science is inherently a human endeavor influenced by the various factors that shape human perspective.

Positivism in Psychology

A group of researchers examining data with different tools, showcasing the evolution of positivist methods.

Modern-day scientists using advanced research equipment is a testament to positivism's lasting impact on scientific investigation

Emphasis on Quantitative Methods and Statistical Analysis

Within psychology, positivism has significantly influenced the emphasis on quantitative methods and statistical analyses. This stems from a desire to achieve objectivity, clarity, and replicability in understanding human behavior. Many psychologists, eager to solidify psychology's reputation as a rigorous science, championed positivist methods.

As a result, controlled experiments, surveys, and standardized tests have been predominantly used, all producing quantifiable data that can be statistically analyzed to discern patterns and predict outcomes. Tools such as the IQ test and the Big Five Personality Traits assessment epitomize this positivist influence, simplifying complex human attributes into numerical scores.

Systematic Approach to Research

One of the most commendable contributions of positivism to psychology is its systematic research approach. Positivism has allowed researchers to systematically investigate various psychological phenomena by emphasizing replicable experiments and observation-based evidence. This methodological rigor ensures that findings are not mere results of chance or subjective interpretation but are rooted in consistent patterns and reliable data. The demand for rigorous and objective investigation pushes researchers to ensure their studies can withstand scrutiny and be replicated under similar conditions, leading to more reliable conclusions about human behavior and cognition.

Advancement in Standardized Methodologies

With the positivist approach, psychology witnessed an evolution in its methodological tools. Standardized methodologies, ranging from structured interviews to psychometric tests, have become staple tools in the psychologist's toolkit. These tools offer a means to measure psychological constructs across various contexts and populations consistently.

For instance, intelligence tests, developed based on positivist principles, aim to consistently measure cognitive abilities across different individuals, minimizing subjective biases. Such standardization ensures that regardless of the researcher or the participant, the results generated have a certain level of comparability and universality.

Formulation of Evidence-Based Theories

Positivism, emphasizing empirical evidence, has given rise to numerous evidence-based theories in psychology. Unlike philosophical speculations, these theories are grounded in observable and measurable phenomena. For example, the theory of classical conditioning, stemming from the works of Pavlov, is a testament to how empirical observations can lead to the development of foundational theories in psychology. These theories, backed by empirical data, provide a solid foundation for subsequent research and therapeutic practices.

Achieving Psychology's Reputation as a Rigorous Science

The adoption of positivist principles played a pivotal role in elevating psychology's standing as a rigorous science. Before the incorporation of empirical methods, psychology was often seen in the same light as philosophy or mere introspection. However, the stringent methodologies and objective frameworks introduced by positivism have solidified its reputation as a discipline rooted in empirical evidence. This reputation has, in turn, facilitated funding, research advancements, and the integration of psychological insights into various domains of societal infrastructure, from education to healthcare.

Critiques of Positivism in Psychology

Oversimplification of Complex Human Experiences

Positivism, with its staunch emphasis on empirical evidence and quantitative measures, is criticized for oversimplifying intricate human behaviors and emotions. While positivist psychology has advanced our understanding of certain phenomena by categorizing and measuring them, there are inherent complexities that this approach can sometimes fail to grasp. For instance, emotions like love, grief, or envy are multifaceted and cannot be easily boxed into a single quantifiable measure. Positivism sometimes assumes linearity in human behavior, neglecting many psychological phenomena's nonlinear and unpredictable nature.

The Challenge of Quantifying Qualitative Experiences

One of the cornerstones of positivism is its trust in observable and measurable data. However, this poses a challenge when the subject matter revolves around experiences that are profoundly personal and qualitative in nature. How does one quantify the depth of a mother's love or the intensity of an artist's passion? Such experiences are inherently subjective and resist the kind of categorization positivism favors. While scales and questionnaires attempt to capture the essence of these experiences, they can sometimes miss the nuances and richness inherent in individual narratives.

Concerns about Reductionism and Ignoring Subjective Accounts

Reductionism is a common criticism levied at positivist approaches. By attempting to reduce complex behaviors and emotions to mere numbers or observable traits, positivism can strip away the richness of human experience. For instance, a positivist approach might reduce depression to measurable symptoms, neglecting the lived experience and subjective accounts of those suffering. This limits the depth of understanding and can result in treatments or interventions that fail to address the underlying causes or individual variations in experience.

By leaning heavily on the empirical, positivism often sidesteps subjective accounts, narratives, and personal stories. These accounts provide a wealth of insight into human behavior and are a cornerstone of many non-positivist approaches in psychology. By discounting or ignoring them, positivist psychology risks missing out on a rich tapestry of human experience and understanding.

Alternatives to Positivism in Psychology

Contrast with Phenomenological, Psychoanalytic, and Humanistic Approaches

While positivism emphasizes empirical observation and quantifiable data, other approaches in psychology delve deeper into the subjective realm of human experience.

The phenomenological approach, for instance, seeks to understand the essence of an individual's lived experiences. It is not concerned with measuring or categorizing these experiences but rather with understanding them from the individual's perspective. Phenomenology considers the nuances of perception, consciousness, and experience, aiming to capture the richness of human subjective experience.

The psychoanalytic approach, introduced by Sigmund Freud, delves into the unconscious mind, seeking to understand the hidden motives, desires, and conflicts that drive human behavior. This approach emphasizes early life experiences and internal conflicts rather than observable behavior. It is a deep dive into the intricate labyrinth of the human psyche, focusing on interpretation rather than quantification.

The humanistic approach emphasizes individual potential and self-actualization. This perspective views humans as inherently good and believes each individual possesses a unique growth potential. It values subjective experiences and individual perceptions, stressing the importance of self-awareness, free will, and personal responsibility. Unlike positivism, which might see humans through a lens of measurable traits, the humanistic approach sees them as whole beings capable of personal growth and self-realization.

The Balance between Quantitative and Qualitative Research Methods

Quantitative and qualitative research methods offer valuable insights into human behavior, and psychology benefits from a balanced integration of both. Quantitative methods, favored by positivism, provide objective, measurable data. They allow for large-scale research, hypothesis testing, and the drawing of general conclusions. However, they sometimes fall short of capturing the depth and intricacy of individual experiences.

On the other hand, qualitative methods prioritize depth over breadth. They capture individual narratives, emotions, and experiences, offering a rich tapestry of insights that numbers alone cannot provide. Through methods like case studies, interviews, and open-ended questionnaires, qualitative research dives deep into individual experiences and perceptions.

In modern psychology, there is an increasing recognition of the value of both approaches. Many researchers advocate for a mixed-methods approach, combining quantitative and qualitative methods, to provide a comprehensive picture of psychological phenomena. By leveraging the strengths of both methods, psychology can achieve a more holistic and nuanced understanding of the human mind and behavior.

While positivism has played a pivotal role in shaping the trajectory of psychological research, it is not the only approach. By integrating insights from phenomenological, psychoanalytic, and humanistic perspectives and balancing quantitative and qualitative research methods, psychology can offer a more complete and multifaceted view of the human experience.

Conclusion

A woman wearing glasses, thinking, overlapped with the image of a galaxy.

History shows us that not singular perspectives but rather integrative ones can guide us toward finding the fundamental truths of our world.

Reflecting on the Legacy and Relevance of Positivism in Today's World

Positivism has played an instrumental role in shaping the modern scientific landscape. Its unwavering commitment to empirical observation and its quest for universal truths have set the benchmark for rigorous research methodologies in natural sciences and across diverse academic disciplines.

In today's complex, multifaceted world, the relevance of positivism remains profound, albeit more nuanced than before. The digital age, characterized by data-driven decision-making, resonates deeply with the positivist ethos. Large-scale data analyses, algorithm-driven predictions, and evidence-based policies underscore a world deeply influenced by positivist principles.

Its Enduring Strengths and Contributions to Scientific Knowledge

The strengths of positivism are undeniable. By supporting a systematic, objective approach, positivism has allowed for identifying patterns, consistencies, and regularities in seemingly chaotic phenomena. Such discoveries have paved the way for groundbreaking innovations, predictive capabilities, and a deeper understanding of the universe's underpinnings.

The achievements of modern medicine, the marvels of space exploration, and the advances in computing are, to a significant extent, the fruits of a positivist approach to inquiry. Time and again, the empirically driven methodologies of positivism have showcased their ability to transcend cultural, geographical, and temporal boundaries, yielding knowledge that stands the test of scrutiny and skepticism.

Positivism’s Lasting Impact on Psychology

Positivism has underscored the importance of observable, measurable facts in the field of psychology. This school of thought asserts that genuine knowledge can only arise from empirical and logical foundations rather than metaphysical speculations. Hence, it paved the way for psychology to transition from a purely philosophical domain to one grounded in empirical research.

Even today, positivism’s principles remain influential in guiding researchers. The emphasis on empirical evidence ensures that studies are reliable, valid, and can be replicated. Such a rigorous approach has led to breakthroughs in understanding human behavior, cognition, and emotion.

However, like all theories, positivism has faced criticism. Some scholars argue that not all psychological phenomena can be measured or observed directly. Yet, the enduring influence of positivism cannot be denied. It has set a benchmark for empirical rigor, leading to more nuanced methodologies that consider measurable and subjective human experience aspects.

To truly understand the depths of concepts like positivism and its applications in psychology, acquiring a formal education can be of great help. Delving into intricate theories, methodologies, and their interconnectivity requires structured learning and guidance.

Our Psychology Program at Meridian University is tailored to address these needs. It integrates rather than separates the myriad perspectives from different cultures, philosophies, and science paradigms.

The program allows you to see the interconnected nature of these approaches and how to use them to explore the true nature of the human mind.

Start your journey by emailing an Admission Advisor to determine whether our curriculum aligns with your academic and professional aspirations.

References

  1. Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. (2019). Positivism and the separation of law and morals. Retrieved from link

  2. Tamanaha, B. Z. (1997). Realistic socio-legal theory: Pragmatism and a social theory of law. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  3. Smith, L. D. (2008). Behaviorism and logical positivism: A reassessment of the alliance. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  4. Mill, J.S. (1865). Auguste Comte and Positivism. London: Trübner & Co.

  5. Hart, H.L.A. (1961). The Concept of Law. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

  6. Carnap, R. (1934). The Unity of Science. London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner & Co.

  7. Comte, A. (1848). A General View of Positivism. Paris: Robert Laffont.

  8. Ayer, A.J. (1936). Language, Truth, and Logic. London: Gollancz.

  9. Durkheim, E. (1895). The Rules of Sociological Method. Paris: Alcan.

  10. Bacon, F. (1620). Novum Organum. London: Billium.

  11. Coolican, H. (2014). Research methods and statistics in psychology. London: Psychology Press.

  12. Smith, J. A. (2015). Qualitative psychology: A practical guide to research methods. London: Sage.

  13. Schultz, D. P., & Schultz, S. E. (2016). A history of modern psychology. Cengage Learning.

  14. Carnap, R. (1959). The logical syntax of language. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.

  15. Hempel, C. G. (1951). Fundamentals of Concept Formation in Empirical Science. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

  16. Popper, K. R. (1959). The Logic of Scientific Discovery. London: Hutchinson.

  17. Hawthorn, G. (1991). Plausible Worlds: Possibility and Understanding in History and the Social Sciences. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  18. Murphy, J. (1990). Pragmatism: From Peirce to Davidson. Boulder: Westview Press.

  19. Bhaskar, R. (1978). A Realist Theory of Science. Leeds: Leeds Books.

  20. Kuhn, T. S. (1962). The Structure of Scientific Revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

  21. Stanovich, K. E. (2010). How to think straight about psychology. Boston, MA: Pearson.

  22. Nunnally, J. C., & Bernstein, I. H. (1994). Psychometric theory (3rd ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill.

  23. Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned reflexes. London: Oxford University Press.

  24. Leahey, T. H. (2004). A history of psychology: Main currents in psychological thought (6th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson/Prentice Hall.

  25. Smith, J. A. (2010). Interpretative phenomenological analysis: A reply to Amedeo Giorgi. Existential Analysis, 21(2), 186-192.

  26. Jones, R. E., & Elcock, J. (2011). History and Theories of Psychology: A Critical Perspective. Academic Press.

  27. Denzin, N. K. (2009). The research act: A theoretical introduction to sociological methods. Transaction publishers.

  28. Moustakas, C. E. (1994). Phenomenological research methods. Sage Publications, Inc

  29. Freud, S. (1923). The ego and the id. WW Norton & Company.

  30. Rogers, C. R. (1961). On becoming a person: A therapist's view of psychotherapy. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.

  31. Creswell, J. W., & Poth, C. N. (2018). Qualitative inquiry and research design: Choosing among five approaches. Sage publications.

  32. Kitchin, R. (2014). The Data Revolution: Big Data, Open Data, Data Infrastructures and Their Consequences. London: Sage.

  33. Sismondo, S. (2010). An Introduction to Science and Technology Studies. Wiley-Blackwell.

Learn More

Interested in learning more about the programs at Meridian?

Contact An Advisor »Attend an Info Session »
learn more about meridian xllearn more about meridian lglearn more about meridian mdlearn more about meridian sm

San Francisco Bay Area Center

47 Sixth Street
Petaluma, California 94952
+1 (707) 765-1836

Los Angeles: Water Garden Campus

2450 Colorado Avenue
Santa Monica, California 90404
+1 (310) 876-2001

Athens Center

Ermou 56
Athens 10563, Greece
+30 21 1199 0060

Berlin Center

Greifswalder Strasse 226
10405 Berlin, Germany
+49 30 16637734

Johannesburg Center

Atrium on 5th 9th Floor
5th St, Johannesburg, 2196, South Africa
+27 31 822 9032

Istanbul Center

Mecidiyeköy Mahallesi | Lati Lokum Sokak
Çınar Apt. No: 12, Kat: 4, D: 14
Şişli/İstanbul 34387
+1 (833) 256-2295

Meridian University Logo